Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning
Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_339
Norbert M. Seel
Animal Intelligence: Schemata for Ordering Learning Classes

Zhanna ReznikovaContact Information

(1)  Laboratory of Behavioural Ecology of Animal Communities, Institute of Systematics and Ecology of Animals, Siberian Branch RAS and Novosibirsk State University, Frunze 11, 630091 Novosibirsk, Russia

Contact Information Zhanna Reznikova
Email: zhanna@reznikova.net

Without Abstract

Synonyms

Animal cognition; Animal rationality; Animal reasoning; Animal thinking


Definition

Animal learning and intelligence define how non-human beings solve their living problems based on their individual and social experience. Learning performs adaptive tuning to a changeable environment, and intelligence helps animals to use their learned experiences in new situations. Individual adaptive behavior involves different kinds of learning together with innate behavioral patterns. Classification of learning classes involves basic forms of learning. The modern schema for ordering learning classes makes it easier to work with different forms of learning in animals in comparison with humans and artificial agents.


Theoretical Background

The rise of scientific study of animal intelligence may be portrayed as progressive changes in experimental methods. The development of objective methods of analysis of animal intelligence is attributed to researches studying animal mind in the nineteenth century, based on Darwin’s evolutionary ideas. In 1870, D. Spalding (1873) experimentally investigated innate and learned behaviors in birds and mammals, and J. Lubbock (1882) was one of the first to introduce apparatus and quantification into the study of animal intelligence. Apart from being a powerful stimulus to the development of experimental investigations, Darwin’s ideas of succession in animal and human thinking gave new arguments for anthropomorphic approach to animal intelligence, and the most known example is the G. Romanes’ book “Animal Intelligence” (1881). The predominance of anecdotal evidence of animal intelligence led one of the pioneers of comparative psychology, L. Morgan (1896), to construct the idea of animal intelligence based on quantitative studies of animals’ reactions to different stimuli. Morgan’s lecture on habit and instinct in animals prompted E.L. Thorndike (1911) to elaborate a novel experimental approach based on the study of animals escaping from puzzle boxes. At the beginning of the twentieth century, two scientific schools that approached learning basing on insight (Gestaltism) and on conditioning (behaviorism, with its Pavlovian and Skinnerian branches) had started almost simultaneously on their efforts to describe learning quantitatively and objectively. After half a century of battles, W. Köhler (1959) invited students of animal intelligence to “forget about schools” and proceed in another direction. The coherent development of ethology and experimental comparative psychology has resulted in cognitive ethology, that is, the comparative, evolutionary, and ecological study of animal minds, including rationality, information processing, and consciousness. Revolutionary experimental paradigms have been developed for studying animal “linguistic” capacities, numerical competence, abilities for rule extraction, sophisticated tool use, complex forms of communication, social learning, and social navigation (for a detailed review see: Reznikova 2007).


Important Scientific Research and Open Questions

Animal intelligence has been experimentally studied for not much longer than a century and controversial ideas still exist about how animals learn and to what limits they understand relations between things and their properties, as well as relations between members of their social groups. Many elegant experimental schemes have been elaborated for investigating how complex are the problems that animals are able to solve. However, there is no common metric for measuring animal intelligence. There is a growing body of evidence that members of different species can solve very complex problems but their cognitive abilities lie within a narrow domain of “species genius.” For example, ants appeared to be more competent (Reznikova and Ryabko 2011) than chimpanzees (Beran 2009) in numerosities, and New Caledonian crows (Kacelnik et al. 2004) are even more advanced than chimpanzees (McGrew 2004) in tool manufacture. There is much work to be done to extend our understanding of whether at least some species share advanced characteristics of intelligence with human beings, or whether all animals think about the world in a way radically different from our own.

To complete the multifaceted panorama of animal intelligence, the working schema of learning classes is needed that involves recent discoveries in the field.

Since W.H. Thorpe (1963) proposed the labeling system of learning classes, there were several updates following a course of development of cognitive ethology. R.K. Thomas (1996) synthesized a list of eight fundamental types of learning from which any and all examples of learning are derived:
  Level 1 – Habituation or Sensitization.
  Level 2 – Signal Learning (Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning).
  Level 3 – Stimulus–response Learning (Instrumental or Operant Conditioning).
  Level 4 – Chaining (Learning Sequences of Stimulus–response Learning Units).
  Level 5 – Multiple Discrimination Learning: Concurrent Discrimination Learning (CDL) or Learning Set Formation (LS).
  Level 6 – Absolute and Relative Class Concept Learning.
  Level 7 – Using Class Concepts in Conjunctive, Disjunctive, or Conditional Relationships.
  Level 8 – Using Class Concepts in Biconditional Relationships.
A new variant of the labeling system of learning classes that integrates data from cognitive, ethological, and ecological studies was suggested in (Reznikova 2007):
1.  Habituation
2.  Associative learning
(a)  Classical conditioning (Stimulus–Reaction)
(b)  Operant conditioning (Stimulus–Reaction–Stimulus)
3.  Catalog learning (Stimulus–Pattern)
4.  Guided learning
5.  Imprinting
6.  Latent learning and exploration
7.  Learning set formation
8.  Rule extraction
(a)  Classification and categorization
(b)  Concept formation at different levels of abstraction
(c)  Causal reasoning
9.  Social learning
(a)  Social facilitation
(b)  Emulation
(c)  Imitation
(d)  Teaching

It should be noted that whereas latent learning, learning set formation, rule extraction, and social learning can be attributed to cognitive abilities, catalog learning, guided learning (Gould and Marler 1987), and imprinting (Lorenz 1935) are based on innate predisposition to build up one set of associations more readily than another. Among these more or less “pre-programmed” forms of learning, “catalog learning” has been described only recently and means animals’ ability to select quickly and to manipulate readily with innate behavioral patterns. Animals look like “cataloging” their repertoire of innate patterns in order to optimize their response to a certain repetitive event (Reznikova 2007). This is a relatively simple, universal, and quite “natural” form of learning that possibly underlies cognition.

The schema for ordering learning classes aims at completing the picture of interactions between different forms of learning in human and nonhuman mentality, and can be applied in cognitive ethology, comparative psychology, and robotics.


Cross-References

Abstract Concept Learning in Animals

Accounting and Arithmetic Competence in Animals

Categorical Learning

Conditional Reasoning by Nonhuman Animals

Contingency in Learning

Evolution of Learning

Habituation

Individual Learning

Learning Set Formation and Conceptualization

Operant Behavior

Reinforcement Learning in Animals

Social Learning in Animals

Theory of Mind in Animals

Tool Use and Problem Solving in Animals


References

Beran, M. J. (2009). Chimpanzees as natural accountants. Human Evolution, 24, 183–196.
 
Gould, J. L., & Marler, P. (1987). Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 256, 74–85.
 
Kacelnik, A., Chappell, J., Weir, A. A. S., & Kenward, B. (2004). Tool use and manufacture in birds. In M. Bekoff (Ed.), Encyclopedia of animal behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 1067–1069). Westport: Greenwood Publishing Group.
 
Köhler, W. (1959). Gestalt psychology today. American Psychologist, 14, 727–734.
 
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Der Artgenosse als auslösendes Moment sozialer Verhaltensweise. Journal of Ornithology, 83, 137–213.
 
Lubbock, J. (1882). Ants, bees, and wasps. A record of observations on the habits of the social Hymenoptera. London: Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.
 
McGrew, W. C. (2004). The cultured chimpanzee. Reflections on cultural primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
 
Morgan, C. L. (1896). Habit and instinct. London: Edward Arnold.
 
Reznikova, Zh. (2007). Animal intelligence: From individual to social cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
 
Reznikova, Zh, & Ryabko, B. (2011). Numerical competence in animals with an insight from ants. Behaviour, 148(4), 405–434.
 
Romanes, G. J. (1881). Animal intelligence. London: Kegan Paul.
 
Shettleworth, S. J. (1998). Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
 
Spalding, D. A. (1873). Instinct, with original observations on young animals. Macmillan’s Magazine, 27, 282–293.
 
Thomas, R. K. (1996). Investigating cognitive abilities in animals: Unrealized potential. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 157–166.
 
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.
 
Thorpe, W. H. (1963). Learning and instinct in animals (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.